Cognitive functioning
Cognitive outcomes for children with ADEM are seemingly positive, with group-level analyses revealing no significant impairment.4 However, closer inspection of individual-level analyses revealed that 16% to 66% of cases demonstrate an impairment (defined as ≥1 SD below the mean) in at least one cognitive domain.4 In their systematic review of 13 studies, Burton et al
4 found that deficits in attention were the most common (43%), followed by deficits in learning and memory (33%), executive functioning (30%), processing speed (27%), visuospatial functions (20%) and academic abilities (12%). These cognitive deficits have been reported to persist for more than 5 years following an episode of ADEM.14 20 Readers should be mindful, however, that impairments defined as ≥1 SD below the mean (ie, 15% of the population) may not reflect meaningful cognitive deficits, and future research should use more stringent criteria.
Studies exploring attentional functions in ADEM demonstrate deficits on objective standardised performance-based tests as well as informant-report questionnaires.1 2 7 21–23 Attentional functions implicated include selective visual attention,14 sustained attention1 and divided attention22 ; however, these domains are not consistently impaired across studies. Nevertheless, attentional impairments can be quite severe in ADEM and may warrant a diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Illustratively, an Israeli study reported that 44% of their sample of paediatric-onset ADEM fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for ADHD, which was significantly higher than the rate in the general population in Israel.2 Some factors associated with better attentional abilities after ADEM include older age of onset24 and longer follow-up duration.14
Similar to attention, research has also demonstrated deficits in information processing speed. Two studies reported that one-third of their ADEM patients demonstrated impaired (defined as >1 SD below the normative mean) processing speed on testing.1 22 Congruently, Kuni et al
20 found that ADEM individuals performed significantly poorer than controls (Cohen’s d=1.19) on the oral version of the Single Digit Modalities Test,25 a sensitive measure for detecting impairment in MS.26 On the other hand, Jacobs et al
24 found no significant difference between the ADEM group and healthy controls on other measures such as Symbol Search from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Third Edition27 or Trail Making Test – Part A.28
Several studies have assessed learning and memory in ADEM.1 20 21 23 Rostásy et al
21 reported that two of their 12 ADEM participants were rated as having memory difficulties on a standardised parental questionnaire but did not provide further information regarding neuropsychological test results. Other studies have examined memory—more specifically, verbal and visual memory—using neuropsychological measures. Regarding verbal memory, Kuni et al
20 found no significant difference in mean score performance between the ADEM sample and healthy controls on the Word Selective Reminding Test;29 however, 31.6% (n=6) of their ADEM sample fell more than one SD below the normative mean. Similarly, Beatty et al
1 and Hahn et al
23 also found verbal memory impairments in five participants (29.4%) and one participant (16.7%) in their respective samples. There is little evidence on visual memory; one study reported that 47.4% of the ADEM patients demonstrated deficits (defined as >1 SD below the normative mean) and performed significantly lower on a facial memory test compared with healthy controls.20 Overall, preliminary research suggests that a proportion of individuals with ADEM have memory and learning deficits, although further research is required to replicate these results in larger samples. Future research should also endeavour to investigate whether these memory deficits reflect primary deficits in memory or whether they are secondary to the attentional impairments common in ADEM.
Executive functions are higher-order cognitive functions that are involved in activities such as planning, decision-making, problem-solving and emotional and behavioural regulation. These functions emerge in infancy and continue to develop well into young adulthood,30 so it is possible that ADEM can cause significant disruption to its development. That is, emerging functions like executive functions are thought to be particularly vulnerable to brain illness and insult compared with other more established functions such as motor control in children.31 However, the current research shows mixed findings. Studies in ADEM have found mild deficits in cognitive flexibility22 on the Contingency Naming Test,32 33 but not in other aspects of executive functioning (ie, planning/organisation, problem-solving/abstract thinking and generativity).14 20 22 23 Of note, while Hahn et al
23 found that none of their six participants were impaired on neuropsychological tests, two participants were found to have significant difficulties in day-to-day life as reported on a parental questionnaire. It is possible that children with ADEM experience subtle executive difficulties in unstructured ‘real-world’ settings, which go undetected on standardised neuropsychological tests that are administered in a structured and less demanding environment. This would be consistent with the aforementioned increased prevalence of ADHD in children with ADEM.2
Research into visuospatial functions in ADEM is scant but seems to suggest that these functions remain largely unaffected.14 20 22 To date, only one study reported impairments in visuospatial functions in three of their 12 ADEM participants, although the authors did not provide neuropsychological test results.21
Language also seems to remain largely undisturbed with very few cases reporting deficits. Hahn et al
23 reported that none of their participants (n=6) had language impairments, while in Beatty et al.’s1 study, only two of 23 participants (9.5%) performed below the normal range on a vocabulary test. In a larger sample (n=95), five children, including two who were mute, were reported as having abnormal language.5
Regarding global intellectual functioning, studies have reported mixed findings. Suppiej et al
14 found no significant difference in IQ between their sample of 22 participants with paediatric-onset monophasic ADEM (M=108, SD=12) and the theoretical mean (M=100, SD=15). In addition, no participant was found to score below the normal range when assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Third Edition.27 Conversely, Shilo et al
2 reported that 40% of their sample fell below the normal range on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (28% and 12% in the borderline range and impaired range, respectively) and Jayakrishnan and Krishnakumar34 similarly reported that 50% of their sample had borderline IQs. These discrepant findings may be explained by the methodological differences including sample size and statistical power; sample characteristics (eg, Suppiej et al
14 investigated monophasic ADEM cases only); and the use of varying intelligence tests. There has also been some evidence to suggest that age of onset may account for some variation in IQ outcome; Jacobs et al
24 found that their young-onset group (<5 years old), but not the older-onset group (≥5 years old), had significantly lower IQs than healthy controls, although the mean IQ remaining in the average range (M=90.1, SD=12.4).
Psychopathology
Children with ADEM are at an increased risk of psychological problems such as depression and anxiety.1 3 Several factors are negatively correlated with psychological outcomes including lesion volume1 and global intellectual functioning.20 Some research also suggests that a younger age of onset is associated with poorer psychological functioning; Jacobs et al
24 reported that 50% of their young-onset ADEM group indicated clinically significant levels of anxiety symptoms on the Behaviour Assessment System for Children,35 which was more than three times greater than their older-onset group ADEM group. Rates of clinically significant depressive symptoms were also higher in the young-onset group compared with the older-onset group (35% vs 0%, respectively).24
Of note, previous studies have reported differences in symptomatology prevalence across informants. Beatty et al
1 reported that approximately one-third of their ADEM sample were rated as having elevated symptoms of anxiety and depression based on parent reports, while rates were lower on the child self-report measure (17% and 8% for anxiety and depressive symptoms, respectively). Hence, future studies should use gold-standard clinician-rated assessments of mood disorders in addition to using self-report and informant-report screening measures.